Exploring thematic prominence and information focus in FYC writers' development of a scholarly stance:
AAAL 2016
Daniel Kies
Department of English
College of DuPage
Adjacency and precedence
A number of scholars studying language from a functional point of view (cf.,
Danes, 1964; Firbas, 1964, 1966; Halliday, 1985; and Quirk, Greenbaum, Leech, & Svartvik, 1985) have recognized the
role that the linear order of words plays in written texts to compensate for the loss of explicit clues about
information focus available through intonation in the spoken language. In spoken English, emphasis is achieved by
manipulating intonational contours so that informationally important elements receive nuclear stress in tone units.
Since those tone units can be conveyed directly, this creates few problems. In written English, however, those tone
units must be inferred from their graphic representations. Consequently, the reader has to rely more heavily on certain
conventions of interpretation especially conventions that relate the structure of the tone unit to the structure
and linear ordering of the syntax. While speakers can directly convey their emphases through stress and pitch, the
writer must construct a clause, indeed the whole sentence (and text), carefully with judicious word order,
punctuation, and discourse implication (about what the writer considers given or new information for the audience)
to recapture the loss of such explicit markers of emphasis in speech. The loss of prosodic markers of
information focus in written English highlights a major problem that arises for all writers who must rely on word order
to carry information focus. That problem might be called the poverty of surface syntactic information, i.e., the
restricted number of distinctions linear order can make as an information carrying device. Linear order can provide
only two possible pieces of information: (1) two constituents can be sequential or not (i.e., linear order can describe
adjacency relations) and (2) if the constituents are sequential, their order may be either X - Y or Y - X (i.e.,
linear order can describe precedence relations).
The poverty of surface syntactic information
Through the relationships of adjacency and precedence, linear order supplemented
by morphology and intonation (in speech) provides information about grammatical relationships of subject,
object, etc; about thematic structure of theme/rheme or topic/comment (the psychological subject discussed by Sandmann,
1954); about the participant roles of agent, patient, etc (the logical subject of Sandmann, 1954); and about
information structure of given and new (cf., Prince, 1981). As Chafe (1976, p. 27) puts it, "A noun in its
sentence plays many roles, or has the potential of doing so."
So part of the difficulty in determining the contribution of linear order to our understanding of language results
from the interplay of various language processes. Those linguistic processes conspire to determine the linear order of
clausal constituents and the linear order of clauses themselves. Out of context, or in a controlled context, it is
possible to isolate the functions of end-focus, thematic prominence, or euphony in determining linear order, but in
vivo, as it were, it becomes more difficult to characterize precisely the contribution of individual language
processes. Neutralization processes provide an analogous situation to the conspiracy described here. Neutralization
rules, at any level of linguistic analysis, eliminate a potential contrast, thereby creating the potential for
ambiguity. At some level of analysis, one would want to explain the ambiguity by positing different forms, which are no
longer overtly distinguished at the surface level because of a neutralization process. Likewise, different sentence
types seem to neutralize some distinctions between grammatical, thematic, psychological, and logical subject, for
example, in order to express some distinction that otherwise may be missed. That is, in appropriate contexts, language
users may need to be explicitly clear about information structure, or thematic structure, etc, for efficient language
processing. For example, when speakers need to be explicit about the packaging of information within a clause, the
cleft and pseudo-cleft constructions allow a distinction between given versus known information. And as Pawley and
Syder (1983) argue, certain structural strategies (or packages of information) may be better suited in certain
communicative contexts because of physical or cognitive constraints, and many of those constraints vary as context
varies, arguing against any notion of an ideal relationship between form and function.
Conclusion
To compensate for the poverty of surface syntactic information in written English, writers commonly employ two
conventions or strategies of interpretation end-focus and thematic structure. Two phenonmenon that are difficult
to measure in data sets because of their context-sensitive nature, at least while corpus tools tend to be context-free.
A Colloquium for the American Association of Applied Linguistics 2016, Orlando
10 April 2016
© 2016 Daniel Kies. All rights reserved.